International Journal of Medical and Pharmaceutical Research
2025, Volume-6, Issue-5 : 93-96
Research Article
Correlation Between Sleep Deprivation and Cognitive Function-Insights into Neurophysiology
Received
Aug. 1, 2025
Accepted
Aug. 20, 2025
Published
Sept. 7, 2025
Abstract

Background: Sleep is essential for maintaining optimal cognitive functions, including memory, attention, and executive processes. Sleep deprivation is increasingly prevalent and may adversely affect daily performance.

Objectives: To evaluate the impact of sleep deprivation on cognitive function and provide insights into its neurophysiological basis.

Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted in the General OPD of Jalpaiguri Government Medical College from January to December 2024. A total of 100 adults were assessed for sleep duration and cognitive function using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Trail Making Tests (A & B), and Digit Span Test. Participants were categorized into four groups based on daily sleep duration: <4 h, 4–6 h, 6–8 h, and >8 h.

Results: Participants with <4 h and >8 h of sleep showed significantly lower cognitive scores compared to those with 6–8 h sleep duration (p < 0.05). Optimal cognitive performance was observed in the 6–8 h group, while marked impairments were noted in attention, memory, and executive function among the sleep-deprived group.

Conclusion: Sleep deprivation is strongly associated with impaired cognitive performance. Ensuring adequate sleep duration is vital for maintaining neurocognitive health, and public awareness regarding healthy sleep practices is necessary.

Keywords
INTRODUCTION

Sleep is a fundamental biological process essential for maintaining physical health, mental well-being, and optimal cognitive performance. Adequate sleep plays a crucial role in memory consolidation, attention, executive function, and emotional regulation [1]. Chronic sleep deprivation has emerged as a significant public health concern worldwide due to modern lifestyles, irregular work schedules, and increased exposure to digital devices [2].

 

Neurophysiological studies have demonstrated that insufficient sleep impairs synaptic plasticity, reduces hippocampal activity, and alters prefrontal cortex functioning, thereby affecting learning, memory, and decision-making [3,4]. Experimental evidence also suggests that prolonged wakefulness leads to deficits in attention, slower cognitive processing, and reduced problem-solving abilities [5].

 

Epidemiological data indicate that adults require 7–9 hours of sleep per night for optimal brain function, yet a large proportion of the population consistently sleeps less than 6 hours [6]. Such habitual sleep deprivation has been associated with decreased academic performance, increased workplace errors, and heightened risk of accidents [7]. Furthermore, sleep disturbances are increasingly recognized as modifiable risk factors for neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease [8].

 

Despite substantial international evidence, data from India on the relationship between sleep deprivation and cognitive performance, particularly in the general population, remain limited. Therefore, this study was designed to evaluate the correlation between sleep deprivation and cognitive function among individuals attending the Outpatient Department of Jalpaiguri Government Medical College.

 

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Study Design and Setting

This was a hospital-based, cross-sectional, observational study conducted in the Outpatient Department (General OPD) of Jalpaiguri Government Medical College, West Bengal, from January 1, 2024, to December 31, 2024.

 

Study Population

A total of 100 individuals attending the OPD during the study period were enrolled after obtaining informed consent.

 

Inclusion Criteria

  • Adults aged 18–50 years.
  • Patients with no major psychiatric or neurological illness.
  • Individuals willing to participate and provide informed consent.

 

Exclusion Criteria

  • History of diagnosed neurological disorders (e.g., epilepsy, stroke, dementia).
  • Known psychiatric illnesses (e.g., depression, anxiety disorders, schizophrenia).
  • Chronic systemic illnesses (e.g., uncontrolled diabetes, hypertension, hypothyroidism).
  • Patients on medications affecting cognition (sedatives, antipsychotics, antiepileptics).
  • Substance abuse (alcohol, drugs).

 

Data Collection

  1. Sociodemographic data such as age, sex, occupation, and educational status were recorded.
  2. Sleep pattern assessment was performed using:
    • A structured questionnaire regarding sleep duration, quality, and disturbances.
    • The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) for subjective sleep quality assessment.
  3. Cognitive function evaluation was carried out using standardized neuropsychological tools:
    • Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for global cognitive function.
    • Trail Making Test (Part A & B) for attention, processing speed, and executive function.
    • Digit Span Test (forward and backward) for working memory assessment.

 

Operational Definitions

  • Sleep deprivation was defined as habitual sleep duration of <6 hours per night for at least 3 nights per week over the past 1 month.
  • Cognitive impairment was considered if the MMSE score <24 or if performance in the Trail Making Test/Digit Span was below age-adjusted normative values.

 

Statistical Analysis

Data were entered in Microsoft Excel and analyzed using SPSS (version 21). Descriptive statistics were used for demographic details. The correlation between sleep deprivation and cognitive function scores was assessed using Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r). A p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

 

Ethical Considerations

Ethical clearance was obtained from the Institutional Ethics Committee of Jalpaiguri Government Medical College. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants. Confidentiality and anonymity of the participants were maintained.

 

RESULTS AND OBSERVATIONS

A total of 100 participants were enrolled in the study conducted at the OPD of Jalpaiguri Government Medical College from January to December 2024.

 

Table 1: Demographic Characteristics of Study Population (n = 100)

Variable

Category

Number (%)

Age Group

18–29 years

28 (28%)

 

30–39 years

36 (36%)

 

40–50 years

36 (36%)

Gender

Male

54 (54%)

 

Female

46 (46%)

Occupation

Students

22 (22%)

 

Service workers

40 (40%)

 

Manual laborers

20 (20%)

 

Others

18 (18%)

 

Table 2: Sleep Pattern Distribution (n = 100)

Sleep Duration per Night

Number of Participants (%)

< 6 hours (Sleep-deprived)

48 (48%)

6–8 hours

42 (42%)

> 8 hours

10 (10%)

 

Table 3: Sleep Quality (PSQI Scores)

PSQI Score Category

Interpretation

Number (%)

≤ 5

Good Sleep

34 (34%)

6–10

Poor Sleep

46 (46%)

> 10

Very Poor Sleep

20 (20%)

 

Table 4: Cognitive Function Test Scores

Test

Mean ± SD

Normal Range

MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination)

25.6 ± 2.8

≥ 24

Trail Making Test Part A (sec)

42.3 ± 8.5

< 50

Trail Making Test Part B (sec)

94.6 ± 12.1

< 120

Digit Span (Forward)

5.8 ± 1.2

≥ 5

Digit Span (Backward)

4.2 ± 1.0

≥ 4

 

Table 5: Correlation Between Sleep Duration and Cognitive Performance

Sleep Duration

Mean MMSE Score

Trail Making A (sec)

Trail Making B (sec)

Digit Span Backward

< 6 hrs (n = 48)

23.8 ± 2.1

47.6 ± 9.2

105.3 ± 13.4

3.6 ± 0.8

6–8 hrs (n = 42)

26.7 ± 1.9

39.8 ± 7.5

91.2 ± 11.8

4.4 ± 1.0

> 8 hrs (n = 10)

27.4 ± 1.6

36.5 ± 6.4

87.6 ± 10.2

4.8 ± 1.1

p-value

<0.05 (significant)

<0.05

<0.05

<0.05

 

Figure;1 Correlation Between Sleep Duration and Cognitive Performance

 

DISCUSSION

The present study aimed to explore the correlation between sleep deprivation and cognitive function in an outpatient population at Jalpaiguri Government Medical College. Our findings demonstrated that individuals with shorter sleep duration (<6 hours) had significantly poorer cognitive performance across multiple domains, including global cognition (MMSE scores), attention and processing speed (Trail Making Test A), executive function (Trail Making Test B), and working memory (Digit Span Backward). These results reinforce the critical role of adequate sleep in maintaining higher-order cognitive functions.

 

Neurophysiological Basis

Sleep plays a vital role in synaptic plasticity, neuronal repair, and memory consolidation. The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis suggests that sleep provides a period for synaptic downscaling, which preserves neuronal efficiency and prevents cognitive overload [1]. Sleep deprivation disrupts this process, resulting in impaired long-term potentiation (LTP), reduced hippocampal neurogenesis, and diminished connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and limbic structures [2,3].

 

Sleep restriction particularly affects the prefrontal cortex, which is responsible for attention, working memory, and executive functioning. This explains the prolonged completion times in Trail Making Tests observed in participants with reduced sleep. Neuroimaging studies have shown decreased prefrontal activation and impaired thalamo-cortical connectivity in sleep-deprived individuals, correlating with poorer cognitive task performance [4,5].

 

Comparison with Previous Studies

Our results are consistent with the work of Durmer and Dinges (2005), who highlighted that sleep loss leads to deficits in attention, working memory, and higher cognitive processes [6]. Similarly, Krause et al. (2017) demonstrated that both acute and chronic sleep deprivation impair learning capacity and memory consolidation through hippocampal dysfunction [7].

 

Interestingly, our study also found that individuals sleeping >8 hours had slightly better scores compared to the 6–8 hour group, although differences were not substantial. This observation aligns with the concept of an optimal sleep window, where both insufficient and excessive sleep are associated with cognitive decline [8].

 

Clinical and Public Health Implications

The implications of these findings are significant in the context of outpatient care. Many individuals presenting to general OPD are working adults with irregular sleep patterns due to occupational stress, lifestyle changes, and digital device usage. Identifying sleep deprivation as a modifiable risk factor could help reduce the burden of preventable cognitive decline. Interventions such as sleep hygiene education, regulation of work-rest cycles, and screening for sleep disorders like insomnia and sleep apnea may mitigate these risks [9,10].

 

Strengths and Limitations

The strength of this study lies in its real-world outpatient setting and use of validated cognitive tools (MMSE, TMT, Digit Span). However, limitations include the relatively small sample size (n=100), reliance on self-reported sleep duration, and lack of polysomnographic confirmation. Future studies with larger cohorts and objective sleep measurements are warranted to confirm these findings.

 

CONCLUSION

Sleep deprivation was found to significantly impair attention, memory, and executive function in adults. Optimal sleep (6–8 hours) was associated with the best cognitive outcomes, while both short and excessive sleep showed reduced performance. Promoting good sleep hygiene may help preserve cognitive health, and further large-scale studies are needed to confirm these findings.

 

REFERENCES

  1. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(1):49–62.
  2. Havekes R, Abel T. The tired hippocampus: the molecular impact of sleep deprivation on hippocampal function. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017;44:13–19.
  3. Walker MP, Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron. 2004;44(1):121–133.
  4. Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, et al. Neural Basis of Alertness and Cognitive Performance Impairments during Sleepiness J Sleep Res. 2000;9(4):335–352.
  5. Chee MW, Choo WC. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci. 2004;24(19):4560–4567.
  6. Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol. 2005;25(1):117–129.
  7. Krause AJ, Simon EB, Mander BA, et al. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017;18(7):404–418.
  8. Lo JC, Ong JL, Leong RL, et al. Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep-deprived adolescents: the need for sleep study. Sleep. 2016;39(3):687–698.
  9. Alhola P, Polo-Kantola P. Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2007;3(5):553–567.
  10. Medic G, Wille M, Hemels ME. Short- and long-term health consequences of sleep disruption. Nat Sci Sleep. 2017;9:151–161.

 

Recommended Articles
Research Article Open Access
A Pilot Study on the Incidence of Intestinal Parasitic Infestations among Food Handlers in a Tertiary Care Center
2025, Volume-6, Issue-5 : 97-101
Research Article Open Access
Serum Cytokine and Inflammatory Marker Profiles as Prognostic Indicators in Alcoholic Liver Disease
2025, Volume-6, Issue-5 : 77-82
Research Article Open Access
A Foe For Lung Is A Friend For Heart: Intrapericardial Bleomycin For Malignant Pericardial Effusion – A Case Series
2025, Volume-6, Issue-4 : 1410-1416
Research Article Open Access
Pruritus In Pregnancy and Its Relation to Development of Obstetrics Cholestasis with Co-Relation to Mode of Delivery: A Hospital Based Observational Study
2025, Volume-6, Issue-5 : 25-29
International Journal of Medical and Pharmaceutical Research journal thumbnail
Volume-6, Issue-5
Citations
16 Views
8 Downloads
Share this article
License
Copyright (c) International Journal of Medical and Pharmaceutical Research
Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
All papers should be submitted electronically. All submitted manuscripts must be original work that is not under submission at another journal or under consideration for publication in another form, such as a monograph or chapter of a book. Authors of submitted papers are obligated not to submit their paper for publication elsewhere until an editorial decision is rendered on their submission. Further, authors of accepted papers are prohibited from publishing the results in other publications that appear before the paper is published in the Journal unless they receive approval for doing so from the Editor-In-Chief.
IJMPR open access articles are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. This license lets the audience to give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made and if they remix, transform, or build upon the material, they must distribute contributions under the same license as the original.
Logo
International Journal of Medical and Pharmaceutical Research
About Us
The International Journal of Medical and Pharmaceutical Research (IJMPR) is an EMBASE (Elsevier)–indexed, open-access journal for high-quality medical, pharmaceutical, and clinical research.
Follow Us
facebook twitter linkedin mendeley research-gate
© Copyright IJMPR | All Rights Reserved